More targeted thoughts:
- I was definitely intrigued by the constrained variation observed in the stress patterns of English nouns and verbs together. Ross' generalization seems to describe it well enough (primary stress for nouns is further to the left than primary stress for verbs), but that doesn't explain where this preference comes from - it certainly seems quite arbitrary. Presumably, it could be an accident of history that a bunch of the "original" nouns happened to have that pattern while the verbs didn't, and that got passed along through the generations of speakers. The authors mention something later on about how nouns appear in trochaic-biasing contexts, while verbs appear in iambic-biasing contexts (based on work by Kelly and colleagues). This again seems like the result of some process, rather than the cause of it. Maybe it has something to do with the order of verbs and their arguments? I could imagine that there's some kind of preference for binary feet where stress occurs every other syllable, and then the stress context for nouns vs. verbs comes from that (somehow)...
- The authors mention that falling frequency (rather than low frequency) seems to be the trigger for change to {1,2}. This means that something could be highly frequent, but because its frequency lessens some (maybe lessens rapidly?), change is triggered. That seems odd to me. Instead, it seems more likely that both falling frequency and low frequency might be caused by the same underlying something, and that's the something that triggers change. (Caveat: I haven't read the work the authors mentioned, so maybe it's laid out more clearly there.) However, they restate it again at the end of this paper, relating to the last model they look at.
- The last model the authors explore (coupling by priors + mistransmission) is the one that does best at matching the desired behaviors, such as changing to {1,2} more often. I interpreted this model as something like the following: If enough examples are heard, the mistransmission bias encourages mis-hearing in the right direction, given the priors that come from the lexicon on overall stress patterns. However, the mistransmission also means that it goes towards that {1,2} pattern more slowly, so only higher frequencies can make it happen the way we want it to (and this is how it differs from the fourth model that just has coupling by priors).
~~~
References
~~~
Lightfoot, D. (1999). The development of language: Acquisition, change, and evolution. Oxford, Eng-
land: Blackwell.
Lightfoot, D. (2010). Language acquisition and language change. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 677-684. doi: 10.1002/wcs.39.
Pearl, L. & Weinberg, A. (2007) Input Filtering in Syntactic Acquisition: Answers from Language Change Modeling, Language Learning and Development, 3(1), 43-72.
land: Blackwell.
Lightfoot, D. (2010). Language acquisition and language change. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 677-684. doi: 10.1002/wcs.39.
Pearl, L. & Weinberg, A. (2007) Input Filtering in Syntactic Acquisition: Answers from Language Change Modeling, Language Learning and Development, 3(1), 43-72.
No comments:
Post a Comment